andallthat.co.uk
  • Blog
    • SubBlog
  • America 1789-1900
  • MeetTheHistorians
  • Contact

PRIMER: Why should I care about research-based progression models?

4/5/2016

1 Comment

 
This is a primer post which goes with my upcoming blog on progression in history. You can find the main post HERE. You can also find my other primer post, in which I attempted to do a demolition job on the idea of linear progression models HERE.

Linear progression models are riddled with problems. In this post, I want to focus on one response to the confusions created them: research-based progression models. Let me be clear from the outset, research-based models of progression were not designed as a replacement for National Curriculum levels, rather to address their myriad shortcomings and help teachers to really get to grips with what progression in history looks like.

The best thing to do to understand research-based models of progression is to read Lee and Shemilt’s seminal article in Teaching History 113, “A scaffold not a cage”. Research based models differ from linear models because they take students’ work and understanding as a starting point to describe what improvement in history actually looks like. In the above article, Lee and Shemilt offer some descriptions of what pupils’ thinking about historical evidence looks like. They then divide this into “stages” of development. The upper four stages of their model are given here.

Picture
Similar models have been created to describe pupils’ progression in other second-order concepts.
Research-based models such as this are powerful as they actually describe what students’ thinking in history looks like, rather than relying on vague and generic assertions as in the linear models. They are very good at helping teachers to identify the gaps in pupils’ thinking, their misconceptions, and where they might sit in terms of the progression model.

Research-based models of progression have many uses:
  • They can help teachers to hone in on particular misconceptions held by students about second-order concepts. Teachers might then be able to decide on appropriate actions to overcome such misconceptions in class, feedback, or planning.
  • They might enable teachers to assess where pupils are in their historical thinking in a descriptive manner. For example, one might note that a child was unable to use evidence in context. Of course, the follow question would have to find out whether this was an issue of understanding the concept of evidence, or simply a lack of relevant contextual knowledge.
  • They might be used to show children grasping more complex ideas and therefore progressing in their historical thinking. Teachers might have a mental note, or indeed a markbook note, describing each pupil’s demonstrated abilities and misconceptions in particular second-order concepts.
However, research-based models of progression are also tricky to use for a number of reasons:
  • Even though they define a number of stages, these are not evenly spaced, nor do they apply universally to the thinking of all children. They would therefore be less useful as universal measures of pupils’ thinking.
  • Because they are shown in stages, teachers can confuse these for logical steps of progression. Lee and Shemilt (2003) are at pains to point out that it would make little sense for teachers to try and use these as a ladder to climb, suggesting that they might offer a means to identify where students are. However, the seduction of a set of linear steps is often too much for many school data managers!
  • There is still no real mention of the application of such modes of thinking to historical knowledge. That is to say, a pupil may move to stage 7 in their thinking about historical evidence when studying the slave trade, but only attain stage 6 in their later work on the Industrial Revolution. This could suggest they have regressed in their thinking, or it might suggest that they lack the contextual knowledge to hit the top band.
Research-based models of progression have been incredibly useful in defining how progress happens in historical thinking, however I would argue that they are more useful in a diagnostic sense, something which Lee and Shemilt also note in their work.

Rethinking research-based models of progression: the guidepost approach
An interesting and very relevant variation on research-based progression models has come out of the Canadian Historical Thinking Project. Peter Seixas takes the idea of progression in historical thinking and makes a fundamental shift in how we might use it as a guide to help pupils progress. Instead of creating a series of levels, Seixas identifies a set of guideposts which he argues represent gold standards for pupils to attain in particular second-order concepts (Seixas & Morton, 2012). He also goes on to identify a number of “misconceptions” students might have which prevent them from moving towards these guideposts. An example of his work on causation is given below and a sampler of his work on significance can be found HERE.
Picture
(c) Seixas and Morton (2013)
A good task is to apply this to is the student work I used in my primer blog on linear progression models. Have a look at the essay below (it was a 40 minute, no-notes, timed essay done by a Year 7 pupil). What aspects of causal thinking would you say this pupil had mastered and what misconceptions are evident in their work?
Picture
I don’t know about you, but I was immediately drawn to the fact that this pupil does not discuss underlying factors in much depth at all, seeing the victory as largely due to the actions of key people. There are of course other issues I could pick out, but this would certainly help me write them some meaningful feedback. I want to stress here that I would NOT be showing them the guideposts, nor a “student friendly” version of them, they would be used purely to inform my professional judgment.

Importantly, Seixas argues that the conceptual guideposts non-linear and therefore not designed to be split into smaller steps. The hallmark of progress is that students overcome misconceptions and move towards gold standards. As such, pupils’ progression in Seixas’ model might looks less like a line graph and more like a radar chart (see below). In this visualisation I have tried to illustrate how Seixas’ approach helps us to see where pupils are developing more powerful ideas and overcoming misconceptions in relation to the particular guideposts of causal reasoning. We would be able to build similar visualisation about their abilities in other second-order concepts as well.
Picture
This is a powerful way to dispense with the issues of linear progression. Beyond assessment, the guideposts might be used in a number of other ways by history teachers:
  • To set clear, gold standards for pupils’ historical thinking, un-muddied by attempts to create hierarchies of such thinking.
  • To help design lessons to deal with pupils’ misconceptions about aspects of second-order concepts.
  • To identify targets of historical thinking to frame sequences of work. For example, a teacher may plan a small unit around the idea of unintended consequences in the outbreak of the English Civil War.
  • To create assessments which have a meaningful appreciation of what pupils’ responses might look like and to create appropriate mark schemes for these.
  • To give an overarching structure to curriculum – planning to overcome misconceptions in all second-order concepts over the course of three, five, or even seven years.
  • Tom Morton then spends some time discussing activities which might help move pupils to overcome their misconceptions and develop more powerful ideas in the rest of the “Big Six”.

Not quite free yet!
However, there are still some issues with Seixas’ approach. The most important of these is that it does not recognise fully that a student’s mastery of a second-order concept, such as causation, is contingent on their application to particular historical contexts. That is to say, it is not possible to master the concept of causation in isolation. Historical causation is only relevant in as far as it is applied to historical situations. Seixas does note that “Historical thinking does not replace historical knowledge: the two are related and interdependent.” (2008, p. 6). However, the guideposts approach to historical thinking does not fully engage with this issue (nor again do they attempt to).

Let us imagine I managed to persuade the eminent historian of the Soviet Union, Martin McCauley to write me an essay on the causes of the collapse of the Union. I am sure he would write me an outstanding piece, showing a deep knowledge of the guideposts of causal thinking outlined above. However, if I asked him to write me a causal explanation of the collapse of the Anglo-Saxon Heptarchy, he might do slightly less well (to be honest he might be an expert on this also – I have no idea!). The difference of course would be his knowledge. Whilst he would clearly know that a good explanation would deal with inevitability, unintended consequences, underlying causes, and so on, his knowledge would not necessarily allow him to write such an explanation.

Why should it be any different for children? A student might display excellent understanding of all five causal guideposts in a piece of work in Year 7 on William’s victory at the Battle of Hastings, but fail to show the same mastery in Year 8 when discussing the abolition of the slave trade. This would not necessarily show that they had regressed in their understanding of the second-order concept, but rather that they were unable to apply it fully to the context of Abolition. This would most likely be an issue connected with my mastery of the substantive knowledge of the slave trade. It would focus our attention as teachers on the knowledge deficits we needed to fill. By the same token a causal explanation completed in Year 7 on the reasons for the First Crusade, one completed by a Year 10 and one completed by a Year 13 might well all hit all guideposts for causation, but would hopefully differ in the complexity of the content they commanded. Again, this is a matter of knowledge.

However I do think that the guideposts approach to second-order understanding has a lot of power. I will come on to how we might address the disconnect between it and substantive knowledgde in my next post.
References & Useful Reading
  • Blow, F., 2011. Everything Flows and Nothing Stays. Teaching History, Issue 145, pp. 47-55.
  • Brown, G. & Burnham, S., 2014. Assessment After Levels. Teaching History, Issue 157, pp. 8-17.
  • Counsell, C., 2000. Historical Knowledge and Historical Skills: A Distracting Dichotomy. In: J. Arthur & R. Phillips, eds. Issues in History Teaching. London: Routledge, pp. 54-71.
  • DfE, 2013. Mathematics Programmes of Study: Key Stage 3, London: DfE.
  • Ford, A., 2014. Setting Us Free? Building Meaningful Models of Progression for a 'Post-Levels' World. Teaching History, Issue 157, pp. 28-41.
  • Fordham, M., 2016. Knowledge and Language: Being Historical with Substantive Concepts. In: MasterClass in History Education: Transforming Teaching and Learning. London: Bloomsbury, pp. 60-85.
  • Lee, P. & Shemilt, D., 2003. A Scaffold Not a Cage: Progression and Progression Models in History. Teaching History, Issue 113, pp. 13-23.
  • Lee, P. & Shemilt, D., 2004. 'I just wish we could go back in the past and find out what really happened': progression in understanding about historical accounts. Teaching History, Issue 117, pp. 25-31.
  • Seixas, P., 2008. “Scaling Up” the Benchmarks of Historical Thinking: A Report on the Vancouver Meetings, February 14--15. Vancouver, s.n.
  • Seixas, P. & Morton, T., 2012. The Big Six Historical Thinking Projects. Toronto: Nelson.
1 Comment
dissertation help link
10/20/2020 08:47:25 am

If we study throughout the semester then we’ll never face issues in our both mid and final terms examination. We already prepared for dissertation help in advance and at the time we have no worries to study and prepare.

Reply



Leave a Reply.

    Image (c) LiamGM (2024) File: Bayeux Tapestry - Motte Castle Dinan.jpg - Wikimedia Commons

    Archives

    August 2024
    November 2022
    September 2022
    April 2022
    February 2022
    March 2021
    August 2020
    July 2020
    October 2019
    August 2019
    April 2019
    March 2019
    February 2019
    January 2019
    August 2018
    January 2018
    December 2017
    June 2017
    May 2017
    February 2017
    November 2016
    October 2016
    September 2016
    July 2016
    May 2016
    April 2016
    March 2016
    February 2016
    January 2016
    November 2015
    October 2015
    August 2015
    June 2015
    May 2015
    April 2015
    March 2015
    January 2015
    November 2014
    October 2014
    June 2014
    May 2014
    March 2014
    February 2014
    January 2014
    November 2013
    October 2013
    July 2013
    June 2013
    April 2013
    February 2013
    December 2012
    October 2012
    September 2012
    July 2012
    June 2012
    April 2012
    March 2012
    March 2011
    February 2011
    June 2000

    Categories

    All
    2014 KS3 Curriculum
    Book Review
    BUSK Reading
    Christmas
    Comment
    History Ancient
    History C19th
    History C20th
    History Early Modern
    History Medieval
    History Thematic
    Ofsted
    PGCE
    Rant
    Teachers Assessment
    Teachers Case Study
    Teachers Classroom Management
    Teachers Concepts
    Teachers Conference Notes
    Teachers Curriculum
    Teachers Exams
    Teachers Frideas
    Teachers Government
    Teachers Leadership
    Teachers Misc
    Teachers NQT
    Teachers Pedagogy
    Teachers Planning
    Teachers Progression
    Teachers Purpose
    Teachers Stand Alone Lessons
    Teachers Technology
    Teachers Training
    Teachers Trips
    Tvfilm Reviews79f7bb4075

    RSS Feed

Powered by Create your own unique website with customizable templates.
  • Blog
    • SubBlog
  • America 1789-1900
  • MeetTheHistorians
  • Contact