This is a primer post which goes with my upcoming blog on progression in history. You can find the main post HERE. You can also find my other primer post, in which I attempted to do a demolition job on the idea of linear progression models HERE. Linear progression models are riddled with problems. In this post, I want to focus on one response to the confusions created them: research-based progression models. Let me be clear from the outset, research-based models of progression were not designed as a replacement for National Curriculum levels, rather to address their myriad shortcomings and help teachers to really get to grips with what progression in history looks like. The best thing to do to understand research-based models of progression is to read Lee and Shemilt’s seminal article in Teaching History 113, “A scaffold not a cage”. Research based models differ from linear models because they take students’ work and understanding as a starting point to describe what improvement in history actually looks like. In the above article, Lee and Shemilt offer some descriptions of what pupils’ thinking about historical evidence looks like. They then divide this into “stages” of development. The upper four stages of their model are given here. Similar models have been created to describe pupils’ progression in other second-order concepts. Research-based models such as this are powerful as they actually describe what students’ thinking in history looks like, rather than relying on vague and generic assertions as in the linear models. They are very good at helping teachers to identify the gaps in pupils’ thinking, their misconceptions, and where they might sit in terms of the progression model. Research-based models of progression have many uses:
Rethinking research-based models of progression: the guidepost approach An interesting and very relevant variation on research-based progression models has come out of the Canadian Historical Thinking Project. Peter Seixas takes the idea of progression in historical thinking and makes a fundamental shift in how we might use it as a guide to help pupils progress. Instead of creating a series of levels, Seixas identifies a set of guideposts which he argues represent gold standards for pupils to attain in particular second-order concepts (Seixas & Morton, 2012). He also goes on to identify a number of “misconceptions” students might have which prevent them from moving towards these guideposts. An example of his work on causation is given below and a sampler of his work on significance can be found HERE. A good task is to apply this to is the student work I used in my primer blog on linear progression models. Have a look at the essay below (it was a 40 minute, no-notes, timed essay done by a Year 7 pupil). What aspects of causal thinking would you say this pupil had mastered and what misconceptions are evident in their work? I don’t know about you, but I was immediately drawn to the fact that this pupil does not discuss underlying factors in much depth at all, seeing the victory as largely due to the actions of key people. There are of course other issues I could pick out, but this would certainly help me write them some meaningful feedback. I want to stress here that I would NOT be showing them the guideposts, nor a “student friendly” version of them, they would be used purely to inform my professional judgment. Importantly, Seixas argues that the conceptual guideposts non-linear and therefore not designed to be split into smaller steps. The hallmark of progress is that students overcome misconceptions and move towards gold standards. As such, pupils’ progression in Seixas’ model might looks less like a line graph and more like a radar chart (see below). In this visualisation I have tried to illustrate how Seixas’ approach helps us to see where pupils are developing more powerful ideas and overcoming misconceptions in relation to the particular guideposts of causal reasoning. We would be able to build similar visualisation about their abilities in other second-order concepts as well. This is a powerful way to dispense with the issues of linear progression. Beyond assessment, the guideposts might be used in a number of other ways by history teachers:
Not quite free yet! However, there are still some issues with Seixas’ approach. The most important of these is that it does not recognise fully that a student’s mastery of a second-order concept, such as causation, is contingent on their application to particular historical contexts. That is to say, it is not possible to master the concept of causation in isolation. Historical causation is only relevant in as far as it is applied to historical situations. Seixas does note that “Historical thinking does not replace historical knowledge: the two are related and interdependent.” (2008, p. 6). However, the guideposts approach to historical thinking does not fully engage with this issue (nor again do they attempt to). Let us imagine I managed to persuade the eminent historian of the Soviet Union, Martin McCauley to write me an essay on the causes of the collapse of the Union. I am sure he would write me an outstanding piece, showing a deep knowledge of the guideposts of causal thinking outlined above. However, if I asked him to write me a causal explanation of the collapse of the Anglo-Saxon Heptarchy, he might do slightly less well (to be honest he might be an expert on this also – I have no idea!). The difference of course would be his knowledge. Whilst he would clearly know that a good explanation would deal with inevitability, unintended consequences, underlying causes, and so on, his knowledge would not necessarily allow him to write such an explanation. Why should it be any different for children? A student might display excellent understanding of all five causal guideposts in a piece of work in Year 7 on William’s victory at the Battle of Hastings, but fail to show the same mastery in Year 8 when discussing the abolition of the slave trade. This would not necessarily show that they had regressed in their understanding of the second-order concept, but rather that they were unable to apply it fully to the context of Abolition. This would most likely be an issue connected with my mastery of the substantive knowledge of the slave trade. It would focus our attention as teachers on the knowledge deficits we needed to fill. By the same token a causal explanation completed in Year 7 on the reasons for the First Crusade, one completed by a Year 10 and one completed by a Year 13 might well all hit all guideposts for causation, but would hopefully differ in the complexity of the content they commanded. Again, this is a matter of knowledge. However I do think that the guideposts approach to second-order understanding has a lot of power. I will come on to how we might address the disconnect between it and substantive knowledgde in my next post. References & Useful Reading
1 Comment
10/20/2020 08:47:25 am
If we study throughout the semester then we’ll never face issues in our both mid and final terms examination. We already prepared for dissertation help in advance and at the time we have no worries to study and prepare.
Reply
Leave a Reply. |
Image (c) LiamGM (2024) File: Bayeux Tapestry - Motte Castle Dinan.jpg - Wikimedia Commons
Archives
August 2024
Categories
All
|